Maku language of Auari
Máku | |
---|---|
Maku-Auari | |
Jukude | |
Native to | Roraima, Brazil |
Region | Brazilian–Venezuelan border |
Ethnicity | Jukudeitse |
Extinct | 2000[1] |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | xak |
Glottolog | maku1246 |
Máku ['mako][1], also spelled Mako (Spanish Macú),[1] and in the language itself Jukude, is an unclassified language and likely language isolate once spoken on the Brazil–Venezuela border in Roraima along the upper Uraricoera and lower Auari rivers, west of Boa Vista, by the Jukudeitse[clarification needed]. 300 years ago, the Jukude territory was between the Padamo and Cunucunuma rivers to the southwest.
The last speaker, Sinfrônio Magalhães, died in 2000. There are currently no speakers or rememberers of Máku and no-one identifies as Jukude any longer. Aryon Rodrigues and Ernesto Migliazza, as well as Iraguacema Lima Maciel, worked on the language, and the data was collected into a grammar by Chris Rogers published in 2020.[1]
Name
The people called themselves jukude-itse [zokudeitse][1]: 3 (person-PL) 'people'. When speaking to outsiders, they referred to themselves as [ˈmaku] or [ˈmako]. Maku ~ Mako (in Spanish orthography Macu or Maco) is an Arawakan term for unintelligible languages and people held in servitude in the Orinoco region. (See Maku people for a partial list.) While the stress of the word in other languages called 'Maku' may be on either the first or final syllable, as Máku/Mácu or Makú/Macú (Migliazza, Fabré), the word was pronounced with initial stress by the jukudeitse and so the name is often written with stress on the first syllable: Máku (Dixon & Aikhenvald (1999), Maciel (1991), and Rogers (2020)) or Máko (Campbell 2012),[2] though also Makú or simply Maku (Migliazza). The disambiguator Maku-Auari has also been used.[3]
Genetic relations
Suggested genetic relations involving Máku include:
- with Arawakan
- with Warao
- within a Kalianan grouping with Arutani–Sape (a.k.a. Makú)
- within a Macro-Puinavean grouping with Nadahup (a.k.a. Makú), Katukinan, and Arutani–Sape
Kaufman (1990) finds the Kalianan proposal "promising", though he is now dated.
Language contact
Jolkesky (2016) notes that there are lexical similarities with the Sape, Arutani, and Warao languages, as well as the Saliba-Hodi, Tikuna-Yuri, Katukina-Katawixi, and Arawa language families due to contact.[4]
Phonology
The Máku syllable structure is (C)(C)V(V)(C).[1]: 36
Bilabial | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Glottal | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stop | voiceless | p | t | k | ʔ | |
voiced | b | d | ||||
Affricate | t͡s | |||||
Fricative | ɸ | s | h | |||
Nasal | m | n | ||||
Approximant | w | l | j |
/k/ is voiced to [g] intervocalically and can occasionally be realized as [g] in other environments too.
/j/ is sometimes realized as [ʑ] word-initially before /a u/ or word-medially, as in /jukude/ [ʑokude̞] 'person'. It can occasionally be fronted to [ð] or [z].
Nasals assimilate to the place of articulation of the stop they precede.
/w/ is realized as [ʋ] before /i e/. It is realized as [ʋ] or [ɥ] before /y/ (a sequence which only occurs in the word /lymywy/ 'take'). Rogers (2020) does not state that /w/ is realized as [ʋ] before /ɨ/, but provides the example /wɨtsɨ/ [ʋɨtsɨ] 'mouth'.
/d/ is realized as laminal before [u] – in some words this is in free variation with [d].
/s, ts, n, k/ are palatalized to [ʃ, tʃ, ɲ, c] before /i, y/, while /t, d, l/ become [tʲ, dʲ, lʲ].[1]: 20-26
Front | Central | Back | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
unrounded | rounded | unrounded | rounded | |
High | i | y | ɨ | u |
Mid | e | (o) | ||
Low | a |
Although there exist minimal pairs between /o/ and /u/, some words show free variation between /u/ and /o/, and [o] is an allophone of /u/ in some environments. Rogers (2020) hypothesizes that these patterns are a result of a diachronic sound change in progress and that /o u/ do not represent separate phonemes synchronically.
Apart from the sequences [eo], [au~ao] and [ia], as well as /ai oi/ [aj oj] within a word stem, vowel-vowel sequences are resolved by deleting the first vowel of the sequence, e.g. /teana/ [tana] 'I smell (it)'. The first vowel in a stem may also be deleted in fast speech.
/a/ and /e/ are realized as [ə] when unstressed.
High and mid vowels are lowered word-finally.
Vowels are nasalized following a nasal consonant, /ʔ/ or /h/.[1]: 32-35
Grammar
Máku nouns and verbs inflect for person - either the person of the possessor, on nouns, expressed by a prefix, or of the subject and object, on verbs, which may be prefixes, infixes or suffixes, depending on the verb. There are also suffixes which express plurality of a possessor or subject. The language marks clusivity by distinguishing first person singular from first + second person (inclusive), first + third person (exclusive) and first + second + third person ('unified'). Nouns also inflect for number and case via suffixes. Verbs also inflect for tense, aspect, mood, evidentiality and negation via suffixes.
Noun phrases exhibit the word order possessor possessor-possessed, or noun-modifier. Demonstratives and numerals typically occur before the nouns they modify. There are postpositions which follow nouns.
Intransitive clause word order is typically subject-verb, and transitive clause word order is most commonly SOV. Indirect objects are typically placed after the verb. Phrases which represent new, focused referents may be fronted to the start of a clause or sentence. Any phrase in focus, both nominal and verbal, can take the focus enclitic =ke.
Máku has motion-complement serial verb constructions, with subject-verbal complement-motion verb order.
Coordination is accomplished via juxtaposition, at the phrase and clause levels.[1]: 98-127
Nouns
Máku nouns decline for number, case and possession. Possession is expressed by a prefix, with certain nonsingular possessors adding a separate suffix. Nonsingular number and case are expressed as suffixes. The Máku noun template is poss-stem-poss.nsg-nsg-case, as demonstrated by the following examples.[1]: 41-47
ene
2
e-
2-
tsimala
arrow
Ø-
3SG-
uba
tree
-nuʔu
-SAP.PL
-itse
-NOM.PL
'your [pl.] bows'
uʔsi
other
-itse
-PL
-daj
-COM.PL
'with others'
Possession
The following sample paradigms illustrate the possessive morphemes – note the three-way clusivity distinction, differentiating both 1+2 (inclusive) and 1+3 (exclusive) as well as the case where the speaker, listener and other(s) are included (1+2+3), which Rogers (2020) refers to as 'unified'. The alienable and inalienable possessive paradigms only differ in the expression of the 3sg morpheme. The 1st person inclusive and 3rd person nonsingular forms are formally identical.
Possessor | 'house' (alienable) | 'mouth' (inalienable) |
---|---|---|
1sg | te-mine | te-wɨtsi |
1+2 | tse-mine | tse-wɨtsi |
1+2+3 | tse-mine-nuʔu | tse-wɨtsi-nuʔu |
1+3 | teke-mine | teke-wɨtsi |
2sg | e-mine | e-wɨtsi |
2nsg | e-mine | e-wɨtsi(-nuʔu) |
3sg | e-mine | Ø-watsi |
3nsg | tse-mine | tse-wɨtsi |
The stem change on 'mouth' in the non-3sg possessed forms is one example of an inalienably possessed noun exhibiting a suppletive stem with overt possessive prefixes, of which there are others in Máku.
Note also the following, as an example of an alienably possessed noun with -nuʔu in the 2nsg possessed form.[1]: 45-47
e-
2
meʔkasa
fish
-nuʔu
PL
'your [nsg.] fish'
Number
Number is marked by a nonsingular suffix -itse, which does not depend on animacy. This suffix is not realized for semantically plural referents when the noun is treated as a collective group, or if the noun is modified by a numeral or quantifier.[1]: 42
Case
There are nine attested case suffixes in Máku:[1]: 42-43
Case | Suffix | Example |
---|---|---|
Comitative singular | -siky | Ø-eneʔmu-siky 3SG-brother-COM.SG Ø-lukia 3SG.SUBJ-be.old 'with his older brother' |
Comitative plural | -daj | uʔsi-itse-daj other-PL-COM.PL 'with others' |
Dative, allative | -le | adiata-le place.below-LAT 'downwards' |
Terminative | -ky | waʔpite-ky sky-TERM 'up to the sky' |
Ablative | -leʔni | waʔpite-leʔni sky-ABL 'from the sky' |
Elative, prolative | -waʔni | na-waʔni water-ELA 'on the water' |
Locative, instrumental | -ʔsa | waʔpite-ʔsa sky-LOC 'in the sky' |
Inessive | -wa | tse-watsi-wa 1+2-mouth-INE 'in our [incl.] mouth' |
Temporal | -de | desembru-de December-TEMP 'in December' |
Pronouns
Máku pronouns mark person, number and clusivity.
Person | Singular | Dual | Plural |
---|---|---|---|
1 | tene | – | |
1+2 | – | tsene | tsenenuʔu |
1+3 | – | tekene | tekenenuʔu |
1+2+3 | – | tenenuʔu | |
2 | ene | enenuʔu | |
3 | oje | ojtse |
There are at least two demonstrative pronouns ki 'this' and (a)kwa 'that' - these can take the locative, lative and inessive cases to form demonstrative adverbs.[1]: 50-53
Quantifiers
Numerals may take the classifiers -sy 'period of time' and -ʔnte 'body part', but these appear to be optional.
nukuja
one
-sy
-CLF:time
keʔl-ia
sun-moon
'one day'
The numerals one to four and peʔtaka 'all' are attested to agree in person with nouns they modify if the person is 1sg, 1exc or 2sg.[1]: 59-61
tekene
1+3
teke-
1+3-
synialʔa
three
te-
1-
luke
tall
-na
HAB
'We [excl.] three are tall.'
Verbs
Máku verbs inflect for subject and direct object as well as tense, aspect, mood, evidentiality and negation. Subject agreement can be marked via prefixes, infixes or suffixes, depending on the verb in question, and plural subjects add a suffix. Tense, aspect, mood, evidentiality and negation are expressed through suffixes. Object agreement precedes subject agreement, but is not well known due to lack of data. The relative ordering of the TAME morphemes is also not well known due to lack of documentation, although tense and aspect suffixes are mutually exclusive.[1]: 65,71,80
Subject agreement
Subject | Form | Examples | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
leme 'be red' | inene 'be afraid' | kaj 'stand (transitive)' | kuntsi 'wash' | ku 'see' | ||
1sg | te | te-leme | i<te>nene | te-kaj | ku<te>tsi | ku-te |
1+2du | tse | tse-leme | – | – | ku<tsi>tsi | – |
1+2pl | tse-nuʔu | tse-leme-nuʔu | i<tse>nene-nuʔu | tse-kaj-nuʔu | – | ku-tse-nuʔu |
1+3 | teke | teke-leme | i<teke>nene | teke-kaj | ku<teki>tsi | ku-teke |
1+2+3 | te-nuʔu | – | i<te>nene-nuʔu | te-kaj-nuʔu | ku<te>tsi-nu | – |
2sg | ke | e-leme | i<ke>nene | ke-kaj | – | ku-seke |
2nsg | ke-nuʔu | e-leme-nuʔu | i<ke>nene-nuʔu | ke-kaj-nuʔu | – | – |
3sg | Ø | Ø-leme | i<Ø>nene | Ø-kaj | kun<Ø>tsi | ku-seke |
3nsg | tse(-pu) | tse-leme-pu | i<tse>nene | tse-kaj-pu | ku<tsi>tsi | kula-Ø |
The -pu in the 3pl subject marker is optional and indicates collectivity of an action.[1]: 66
Tense
There are five tense morphemes in Máku: distant past, recent past, present, near future, and distant future. While the tense boundaries are relative and not absolute, distant past is used just for mythological stories, and generally the distant future refers to events after the current day while the near future refers to events later in the day.[1]: 71-74
Tense | Suffix |
---|---|
Distant past | -mutsa |
Near past | -nka |
Present | -Ø |
Near future | -ba |
Distant future | -diba |
Aspect
Máku has six aspect morphemes.
Aspect | Suffix |
---|---|
Permanent | -na |
Temporary | -sia |
Imperfective | -dbena |
Perfective | -dia |
Permanent progressive | -dkina |
Temporary progressive | -dkisia |
The permanent aspect suffix -na is used only with stative verbs and marks permanent and inherent properties.
The temporary aspect suffix -sia indicates temporary properties when used with stative verbs, and unfinished action with active verbs.
The progressive aspect suffixes are present tense in meaning.[1]: 74-76
Mood
Mood | Suffix |
---|---|
Indicative | -Ø |
Imperative | -kɨ(se) |
Hortative | -kada |
Conditional | -wake |
Purposive | -bana |
Verbs marked for the imperative do not have an explicit subject marker. However the hortative does agree with subject.
The conditional mood only occurs in multiclause constructions. However, the purposive can occur both in multiclause and monoclausal sentences, as shown in the below examples.[1]: 76-77
oja-ke
3-FOC
Ø-ja-dia
3-lay.down-PFV
Ø-we-bana
3-sleep-PURP
'He lays down to sleep.'
te-
1SG-
we
sleep
-bana
-PURP
'I will sleep.'
Evidentiality
Evidentiality | Suffix |
---|---|
Firsthand | -tsa |
Non-firsthand | -nia |
The evidentiality suffixes are only used with past tense and are not obligatory.[1]: 78-80
Negation
Negation is signified by -ʔV, where the value of the vowel is the vowel before the suffix, unless followed by -bala, in which case it is /a/.[1]: 80-83
Transitive active verbs can optionally take the additional negative suffix -bala following -ʔV.
-ʔV can also be used as a prohibitive marker, as in
ke-
2-
we
sleep
-ʔe
-NEG
'Don't sleep!'
The negative suffix appears to precede the evidential suffixes, which appear to precede the tense suffixes, as in the following:
nyhe
fire
Ø-
3SG-
la
do
-ʔa
-NEG
-tsa
-FIRSTH
-mutsa
-DIST.PST
asajtse
long.time.ago
-ke
-FOC
'There was no fire a long time ago.'
Vocabulary
Loukotka (1968) lists the following basic vocabulary items for Máku.[5]
gloss Máku one nukuzamuké two bãtá three shünãlyá head tsi-gáte eye tsis-kóte tooth tse-um man lásepa water náme fire níheː sun kélé maize lükü jaguar zówi
References
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y Rogers, Chris (2020). Máku: A Comprehensive Grammar. Taylor & Francis.
- ^ Campbell, Lyle (2012). "Classification of the indigenous languages of South America". In Grondona, Verónica; Campbell, Lyle (eds.). The Indigenous Languages of South America. The World of Linguistics. Vol. 2. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 59–166. ISBN 9783110255133.
- ^ Hammarström, Harald (2011). "A Note on the Maco [wpc] (Piaroan) Language of the Lower Ventuari, Venezuela". Cadernos de Etnolingüística. 3 (1).
- ^ Jolkesky, Marcelo Pinho de Valhery (2017) [original version 2016]. Estudo arqueo-ecolinguístico das terras tropicais sul-americanas [Archeo-ecolinguistic study of South American tropical lands] (Ph.D. dissertation) (in Portuguese) (2 ed.). Brasília: University of Brasília.
- ^ Loukotka, Čestmír (1968). Classification of South American Indian languages. Los Angeles: UCLA Latin American Center.
Bibliography
- Campbell, Lyle (1997). American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-509427-1.
- Campbell, Lyle; Grondona, Verónica, eds. (2012). The Indigenous Languages of South America: A Comprehensive Guide. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
- Dixon & Aikhenvald (1999). "Máku", in The Amazonian Languages (pp. 361–362)
- Fabre, Alain (2005), Diccionario etnolingüístico y guía bibliográfica de los pueblos indígenas sudamericanos: Makú [Ethnolinguistic dictionary and bibliographic guide of the South American indigenous peoples: Makú] (PDF) (in Spanish)
- Kaufman, Terrence (1990). "Language History in South America: What We Know and How to Know More". In Payne, D. L. (ed.). Amazonian Linguistics: Studies in Lowland South American Languages. Austin: University of Texas Press. pp. 13–67. ISBN 0-292-70414-3.
- ——— (1994). "The Native Languages of South America". In Mosley, C.; Asher, R. E. (eds.). Atlas of the World's Languages. London: Routledge. pp. 46–76.
- Koch-Grünberg, Theodor (1922). "Die Volkgruppierung Zwischen Rio Branco, Orinoco, Rio Negro und Yapurá" [The Ethnic Group Between Rio Branco, Orinoco, Rio Negro and Yapurá]. In Lehmannn, Walter (ed.). Festschrift Eduard Seler (in German). Stuttgart: Strecker und Schröder. pp. 205–266.
- Maciel, Iraguacema (1991). Alguns aspectos fonológicos e morfológicos da língua Máku [Some phonological and morphological aspects of the Máku language] (Thesis) (in Portuguese). Universidade de Brasília.
- Migliazza, Ernesto (1965). "Fonología Makú", Boletim do MPEG. Antropología 25:1–17.
- ———— (1966). "Esbôço sintático de um corpus da língua Makú", Boletim do MPEG. Antropología 32:1–38.
- ———— (1978). "Makú, Sapé and Uruak languages. Current status and basic lexicon", AL 20/3:133–140.
- Rogers, Chris (2020). Máku: A Comprehensive Grammar. Taylor & Francis.